Skip to main content
NC3Rs: Macaques logo NC3Rs logo

Enrichment

In this guide
An otherwise plain room has been enriched through the addition of windows, wire mesh and multi-levelled climbing structures

About enrichment

Through the provision of enrichment, the quality of captive animal care is enhanced, providing the stimuli necessary for optimal psychological and physiological well-being.
Here you can see an otherwise plain room, enriched through the addition of windows, wire mesh and multi-levelled climbing structures (Image credit: Karolina Westlund).

Environmental enrichment is the process of providing a stimulating environment to promote species-typical behaviour, allow a degree of control and choice in behaviour, and to enhance well-being [1-5]. Environmental complexity and predictability of events are as important as having adequate space [6-9]. A good enrichment programme will incorporate enrichment interventions that satisfy a range of needs in the social, locomotory, sensory, cognitive and food-based domains [10]. Positive interactions with staff may also be considered as enrichment (see Habituation and training).

Why is enrichment important?

The aims of enrichment include [11,12]:

  • To improve or maintain an animal’s physical and psychological health.
  • To increase the repertoire of species-specific behaviours exhibited.
  • To increase utilisation of the captive environment.
  • To prevent or reduce the frequency of abnormal and unwanted behaviours, such as stereotypies.
  • To increase the individual’s ability to cope with the challenges of living in captivity.

By increasing the complexity of the captive environment, the opportunity for choice also increases. Choice is one way in which captive animals exert control over their environment, their behaviour and their captive situation. Control is an important component of welfare and is linked to predictability, which reduces stress [13].

These weaned rhesus macaques are provided with a variety of types of environmental enrichment. For example, they are housed in social groups, have access to fixed climbing structures made of wood and suspended tyre swings, and there is food scattered around the enclosure and in the floor substrate to encourage foraging behaviour (Credit: Wolfensohn & Finnemore 2006).
Two cynomolgus macaques explore the various structures in their enclosure

Enrichment programmes

Enrichment programmes should be goal-orientated, and ideally developed by an enrichment committee that represents all staff interests (e.g. animal care staff, researchers, facility managers).

Designing and implementing enrichment programmes

The S.P.I.D.E.R. Framework, designed by Disney’s Animal Programs, is a simple step-by-step process you can incorporate when designing an enrichment programme [14].

The S.P.I.D.E.R. Framework components

Enrichment safety considerations

The potential dangers of any enrichment item should be carefully considered and assessed before incorporating these stimuli into your enrichment program [11].

Safety precautions to consider include the following:

  • Choking hazards.
  • Strangling hazards.
  • Transmission of disease.
  • Parts that can be used as weapons.
  • The temperament, age, sex or past experience of the animals [15,22-24].
  • Detrimental psychological effects, such as fear [25].

Also see Enrichment gone wrong!

The Shape of Enrichment website has a searchable ‘Safety Database’, providing information on potentially dangerous enrichment items.

Always observe animals with any new enrichment items to ensure that there are no dangers and to be able to intervene quickly if necessary.

  • A mesh ceiling provides an additional surface from which to hang enrichment and climbing structures. Here chains have been inserted into water hosing to reduce the risk of the monkeys’ digits being caught in the links.
    Enrichment hanging from mesh ceiling
In this video, destructible items (cardboard boxes) are a source of novelty and provide an element of control in otherwise rigid housing. Packaging from deliveries is a cheap source of enrichment items but should be made safe, for example, by removing large staples. Boxes can be filled with substrate and small food items (e.g. seeds, cereals) to increase interest.
 A group of macaques, including an infant, huddle together on a shelf in their outdoor enclosure

Social

Macaques are highly social animals. The companionship of other macaques is of vital importance to their welfare in captivity.

Benefits of social housing

Social housing of macaques provides the opportunity for:

  • Performance of species-typical social behaviours, such as allogrooming, huddling and play, which promotes physical and psychological health [27,28].
  • Developing and maintaining social bonds, which can reduce the negative effects of stressful events and allow animals to cope better with change (social buffering) [29].
  • Reducing stereotypical behaviour [15,30].
  • Socially housed macaques manipulate enrichment items more often than individually housed animals [31,32].

Macaque species naturally live in multi-male, multi-female groups of around 10-50 animals and this structure is ideal for breeding colonies. Grouping a single breeding male with several related females and their offspring (harem) is a common practice, as breeding can be controlled and lineages recorded. Stock groups of 10-20 small (2-3kg) macaques of the same sex are often housed together. Macaques used in experiments can be successfully maintained as groups in toxicology, immunology and neuroscience studies, even those with cranial or other implants [12,33,34].

Pair housing is not a natural social structure for macaques and will not work in all circumstances, so as a general rule it is always best to aim for larger groups. However, safe pair formation and subsequent pair-housing techniques have been developed for adult male and female rhesus, cynomolgus, stump-tailed and pig-tailed macaques [35-38]. When macaques are housed in pairs rather than larger social groups, widely spaced bars between the compatible pairs can increase perceived group size and social interactions without risking health or competition for food, or making identification and capture of animals more difficult [39].

Single housing should no longer be considered acceptable for macaques in the laboratory, with exceptions requiring compelling veterinary or scientific justification [12,40]. If single housing is necessary on the recommendation by a trained primate veterinarian or animal welfare officer, it should be for as short a period of time as possible; the isolated macaque should still be able to hear, see and smell group members [41]. Additional resources should be targeted to the care and welfare of such animals to compensate for the absence of social companions.

Housing in stable compatible groups encourages normal cognitive development and maintains health and psychological well-being, through the ability to express natural behaviours. Even large male macaques with head implants can be housed socially, in enriched enclosures, without damage to the devices. In this video, displacement behaviour can be seen between the three males.

Managing social groups

A 2006 survey of US primate facilities revealed that 52% of rhesus macaques and 62% of cynomolgus macaques housed indoors were kept in single cages [42]. There is always concern housing non-human primates in groups in case conflicts between animals arise [43]. However, these can be managed effectively to allow the welfare benefits of social housing [44,45]. Factors contributing to the successful maintenance of harmonious social groups are summarized below [12].

Introductions to conspecifics

Plan introductions carefully:

  • Do not rush introductions – allowing visual, auditory and olfactory contact first, and then tactile contact through a barrier, will allow for better assessment of compatibility.
  • The grade/width of mesh panel between animals in adjoining cages can be gradually increased to slowly and safely increase the area of contact.
  • Introduce single animals, or small groups of 2-3 of animals, at a time – never large groups.
  • Introductions should be started at the beginning of the day and week to allow maximum time for observations and monitoring.
  • Ensure that there is an adequate number of staff available, especially when full contact is allowed for the first time; inform the attending veterinarian.
  • Always have an ‘interference’ plan in place during introductions, including defining the criteria that will be used to stop an introduction and how to separate aggressive animals.
  • Provide distraction objects, such as new enrichment items and scattered food.
  • Provide as much space as possible, and ensure there are areas where the animals can retreat from each other but not become trapped without an escape route.
  • Monitor the animals over a period of time (short-term and long-term; days and weeks) looking for affiliative behaviours (e.g. grooming, resting in contact, lip-smacking: see Behaviour) and evidence of a dominant-subordinate relationship (e.g. mounting).

Steps to implement:

  • Obtain animals in pre-existing compatible groups or pairs and avoid re-grouping.
  • Utilise information from the breeder/supplier about the social history, behaviour, and health of individual animals, previous and current caging, as well as socialisation methods.
  • Consider temperament testing to determine compatibility.
  • Allow individuals the time to resolve minor conflicts and develop bonds.

Managing aggression

Where aggression occurs, assess the situation:

  • Is this a long-term situation?
  • Is the aggression occurring between or within groups?
  • Is there a risk of injury, or are conflicts settled without dangerous physical contact?
  • Is the animal at risk of poor health, due to age or scientific procedures?
  • Macaques have hierarchical societies – are conflicts occurring between high and low ranked animals, or are they a result of animals trying to rise up the hierarchy [46]?
  • Has there been a recent external or internal stressor?
  • Is the aggression linked to particular events (e.g. feeding, weighing), animals or staff?
  • Is the aggression resource related?

Steps to implement:

  • Acknowledge that social hierarchies fluctuate over time and that aggression is a normal behaviour. Aggressive incidents can occur due to stressors such as stress, illness, puberty, breeding season, husbandry activities and disruptions to the hierarchy.
  • Removing members due to conflict can disrupt hierarchies and make it difficult to return these animals to their group.
  • Do not house too many animals for the space.
  • Provide ample 'escape’ routes, refuge and visual barriers.
  • For pairs of macaques, house animals of different ages and/or body weights together [47].
  • Reinforce hierarchies by attending to dominant and confident animals first.
  • Keep records of aggressive incidents – when does it occur, who is involved, during what circumstances.
  • Minimise arousal at feeding times by feeding without delay (or providing a reliable signal before feeding) and scattering food.
Housing design, and the configuration of the internal space, is important for successful social housing of macaques. For example, they should be able to choose to socialize with or avoid, group mates, and to be seen or not seen by neighbours. This can be achieved by provision of visual barriers, such as floor mounted or hanging plastic panels, sacking and wide pipes, or in the case of this breeding centre, the floor of the second tier. (Credit: BFC).
A cynomolgus macaque hangs upside down on a perch fixed to the enclosure ceiling makes use of the 3D space.

Structural

Structural enrichment is important to increase 3D complexity and usable space within enclosures for macaques, and to permit species typical behaviours, such as climbing, leaping and resting in an elevated position.

Suitable objects to provide structure to enclosures include ladders, barrels, tyres, swings, hammocks, platforms, shelves, logs and branches. Such objects need to be attached securely within the enclosure, as the monkeys will jump onto them with force. Some studies suggest that adult macaques may prefer fixed structures to moveable ones [48-51]. It is preferable to use wood, where possible, as it is a softer material than metal or plastic, and can be gnawed.

Below you can find a range of suggested 'recipes' to provide structural enrichment.

Recipe for suspended pipe 1

Materials

  • 1 strip of fire hose
  • 1 carabiner
  • 1 length of PVC pipe
  • 1 metal ceiling hook/ring

Construction

  • Drill a large hole through the piping to thread the fire hose through.
  • The length of the fire hose used will determine how low the structure will hang.
  • The structure can also be hung off the underside of wooden platforms.
  • Plastic balls can also be hung from the end of the pipe.
  • PVC pipe suspended from fire hose for structural enrichment
    PVC pipe (Image: C Kemp/MRC Centre for Macaques)
  • PVC pipe with plastic blocks attached suspended from a fire hose for structural enrichment
    PVC pipe with blocks (Image: C Kemp/MRC Centre for Macaques)

Recipe for suspended pipe 2

Materials

  • 1 length of PVC pipe
  • 1 carabiner
  • 1 length of fire hose
  • Bolts/screws
  • Small plastic horse blocks

Construction

  • Attach several plastic blocks along the length of the PVC pipe at two points for stability
  • Drill holes into the plastic block to allow water to drain during cleaning
  • The PVC pipe can be hung using rings which can then be attached to fire hose and hung in the housing
  • The plastic blocks can also be attached to other substrates, including wooden poles and the walls to create alternate climbing frames

Recipe for fire hose swings/climbing structures

Swings replicate the flexible branches macaques use in nature and can help to keep the animals physically fit. (Credit: MRC Centre for Macaques)
Screw stainless steel rings and clamps into ceilings and solid walls for additional climbing and attachment options. (Credit: MRC Centre for Macaques).

Materials

  • Lengths of fire hose
  • Carabiners
  • Ceiling plate hooks
  • Metal rings

Construction

  • Use different lengths of hose to connect different points of the housing and create climbing pathways with different tension.
  • Cut holes into the ends of the fire hose with enough room to allow for tearing from general use.
  • Several lengths of fire hose can be attached to the same point using carabiners.

Recipe for horse jump climbing structures

Materials

  • Plastic horse jumps
  • Flat wood planks
  • Wooden poles
  • Strips of fire hose
  • Stainless steel pole base plates
  • Long bolts/screws

Construction

  • There are a variety of ways in which the plastic horse jumps can be put together
  • Distance between the horse jumps will depend on the length of the wooden planks and poles – be creative by playing with this distance for a range of structures
  • Using fire hose improves the grip of the bolts and reduces the chances the monkeys can pull them out
  • Attaching the wooden pole on the metal base plate increases the height of the structure and provides stability to a round object
  • Drill holes into the horse jumps to allow water to drain after cleaning
  • Using hardwood will ensure that this structure is long lasting, reducing wear from chewing and water rot
  • Plastic balls and blocks can also be attached to these structures for variety
  • An example of an indoor enclosure with sawdust and bedding on the floor, lots of wooden structures and perches at different levels, and fire hose for swinging and climbing
    An example of an indoor enclosure (Image: C Kemp/MRC Centre for Macaques)
  • An example of fire hose attached to a ceiling in an indoor enclosure by carabiners on a ceiling hook
    Fixing point (Image: C Kemp/MRC Centre for Macaques)

Recipe for toy swings

Moveable objects catch attention and introduce an element of unpredictability. The plastic horse jumps on the floor act as climbing frames and visual barriers. (Credit: MRC Centre for Macaques).

Materials

  • Length of fire hose
  • Carabiners
  • Hooks and rings
  • Plastic barrels
  • Plastic crates
  • Plastic balls

Construction

  • Attach one end of the fire hose to the wall/ceiling/sturdy platform first, then loop it through a barrel or large plastic box and attach the other end
  • Barrels, crates and boxes can also be attached to just one end of the fire hose using a carabiner and allowed to swing freely
  • Drill holes into the bottom of the plastic barrels/boxes/crates/balls to create a drainage point

Recipe for suspended tyres

Materials

  • Forklift tyres
  • Fire hose
  • Carabiners
  • Ceiling hooks
  • Metal rings
  • Hardwood planks
  • Chain

Construction

  • Tyres can be hung as a swing or anchored upright on the floor
  • Use fire hose or chain with a carabiner to hang the tyre from the ceiling
  • Link tyres together to make a ladder or chain
  • Wood can be bolted onto an upright tyre to maintain its position and create stability
Wooden shelves (Credit: BFC).
Climbing on rings (Credit: BFC).
A macaque using a mirror and avoiding eye contact

Sensory

The specific aim of this type of enrichment is to stimulate the macaques’ senses. The captive environment can be barren in comparison to the wild where macaques are exposed to continually changing stimuli. Sensory enrichment can mitigate for this by providing the animal with stimulus that allows it to engage one or more senses (Image credit: Karolina Westlund).

In this section

Vision

Macaques are highly visual animals, with colour vision similar to humans. It is likely that laboratories housing macaques are already providing visual stimulation. You can further enrich the visual experience of captive macaques by providing:

  • A variety of colours within the housing.
  • Visual contact with other macaques (e.g. through the use of mirrors, windows, enclosure wiring instead of walls).
  • Televisions, either displaying aired programs, video tapes, static images, or live feed of other monkeys or care staff [53-57].
  • Pictures or paintings on the wall.
  • Presentation of slides [58].
  • Mirrors inside or outside the cage.
  • Suspended rings in an outdoor enclosure for climbing, in a variety of colours
    Suspended rings for climbing
  • A macaque using a mirror and avoiding eye contact
    Macaque using mirror and avoiding eye contact
Mirrors can improve visibility of activities within the facility, helping to reduce the likelihood of animals being startled by the sudden appearance of care staff. (Credit: MRC Centre for Macaques).
Mirrors are also a way that macaques can inspect themselves and monitor the activities of members of their social group without making direct eye contact. (Credit: MRC Centre for Macaques).
The angle of the mirror mounted on the outside of this enclosure can be adjusted via a mechanism linked to the interior, enabling the occupants to gather information about their surroundings and providing an element of control. External mirrors are safe and easily cleaned. (Credit: MRC Centre for Macaques).

Audition

Macaques have excellent low frequency hearing but can hear sounds up to 40 kHz [59]; humans only hear up to about 20 kHz. Sounds are a great way to stimulate captive animals and enrich the captive environment beyond everyday noises. These can include:

  • Music [60,61].
  • Natural sounds, such as from non-threatening animals, water, or wind.
  • Conspecifics from other groups within the facility.

Playing music or other sounds can help non-human primates adjust to noise and reduce their startle reflex in response to unexpected or loud sounds [12]. However, it is important to carefully monitor the response of the animals for negative effects. Some sounds may be aversive to due to interference with the animals’ hearing range (too high or too low pitch) or because the volume of the sounds make it difficult for the macaques to stay in auditory contact with other groups [62,63].

Olfaction

The olfactory ability of primates has been largely over-looked in science [64] and thus rarely included in enrichment programs. However, the provision of odours can elicit behaviours from monkeys not otherwise observed. Dotted around the enclosure in small quantities at random intervals, odours can elicit investigation, and the animals may even rub themselves against the odour if they find it pleasant or interesting. Olfactory enrichment stimuli can include:

  • Odours of other macaques, such as those found on nesting material or environmental enrichment objects swapped from the enclosure of another group.
  • Perfumes.
  • Oils.
  • Spices and herbs.

Touch

Touch can be stimulated by ensuring that a variety of surfaces are provided within the enclosure. These may include:

  • Wood (ensure that there is no chance of splinters) and gnawing sticks [22].
  • Smooth plastics.
  • Textiles.
  • Naturalised floor covering (e.g. woodwool, straw, hay, paper shavings, sawdust).
  • Waxed cardboard boxes.
  • Nylabone rings and tug toys.
  • Kong toys (large sized).
  • Rattles and shakers.
  • Magazines, telephone directories or other papers.
  • Pool for swimming [65-68].

Swimming pools, and foraging in water

Nylon rod bulldozer.

Swimming pools provide effective enrichment for macaques in the laboratory. These animals show high motivation to manipulate the water surface, immerse themselves, dive, swim and play (including underwater), even in the absence of submerged food rewards (e.g. raisins, nuts, banana chips). Advantages of this enrichment technique are that it is based on a natural behavioural inclination, encourages play rather than food-orientated enrichment, provides exercise, keeps both animals and their enclosure clean, and can facilitate thermoregulation in hot weather.

Food can be scattered on the bottom of a swimming pool, to encourage the animals to enter and dive under the water.

Recipe for foraging in water

Ingredients

  • Pool or deep tub
  • Water
  • Waterproof plastic crate with lid
  • Fruit pieces

Notes

  • If possible, bolt the crate to the floor of the pool/tub.
  • Place the fruit pieces inside the crate and close the lid.
  • Ensure that the water is replaced regularly to maintain cleanliness.
  • The fruit ice blocks can also be used in water foraging enrichment as the bobbing of the water will increase the challenge level.
Large swimming pools are a good way of encouraging social play in groups of young macaques.
Bubble bath has been added to the water in this swimming pool to help keep the skin and hair coat in good condition.
Stock and experimental macaques at this laboratory are group-housed and given access to the custom-made polypropylene pools about once per week to maintain some novelty. The macaques can be seen swimming, dive-bombing and wrestling.
Cynomolgus macaques diving into a swimming pool, keep your eye on the macaque directly under the camera! (Credit: BFC).

Taste

If your feeding program includes varied food items presented in interesting ways (see Food-based), it is likely that you are already covering taste in your sensory enrichment. Some manufacturers add flavourings and vary the colour, texture, size and shape of their pellets to make them more interesting and palatable. Sweet and fruity tastes generally enhance dietary palatability.

Providing a variety of food types can prevent ‘the monotony effect’ (i.e. reduced food intake) that occurs with prolonged feeding of any one food, and can help maintain appetite in sick animals.

The provision of different textured foods can also act as touch-based sensory enrichment; for example, presenting food in ice blocks. Ice-bound food will help to stimulate multiple senses and extend feeding times, as the monkeys must work harder to access the food.

Recipe for fruit ice blocks

Ingredients

  • Water
  • Bucket
  • Paper cups
  • Fruit pieces
  • Yoghurt
  • Forage mix
  • Peanuts
  • Fire hose
  • Carabiner

Notes

  • Ice blocks can be made using paper cups or buckets, with either water (buckets) or yoghurt (paper cups).
  • Mix chopped fruit pieces in with the water/yoghurt in the container.
  • When using buckets, suspend a piece of fire hose with a carabiner in the water before freezing.
  • Remove the fruit ice blocks once frozen.
  • Hang from a suspension hook or place around the housing.
Paper cups and plastic beakers can be filled with diluted fruit juice and frozen. Obtaining access to the flavoured ice, and eating it, occupies the animals for longer than non-frozen drinks.
Ice cone.
A macaque works on a puzzle feeder that is a ball suspended in the air with small holes in it for macaques to retrieve treats from.JPG

Cognitive

Introducing cognitive challenges into captive environments for macaques is a potential means of enhancing their welfare through the enriching effects of problem solving. Opportunities for promoting problem solving skills include puzzles, computer-based cognitive tasks, and training.

Puzzles

Puzzles can integrate rewards, such as food treats.

  • Example of a puzzle feeder made from plastic where monkeys are required to swivel pieces of plastic around to access food
    Puzzle feeder
  • A macaque works on a puzzle feeder that is a ball suspended in the air with small holes in it for macaques to retrieve treats from.JPG
    A macaque works on a puzzle feeder
Retrieving treats with a pole. Watch how this macaque obtains the food treats that are initially out of reach.

Computer-based cognitive tasks

Computer-based cognitive tasks are a way to stimulate higher processing [70,71].

 

This video shows the whole sequence of a macaque on a neuroscience study leaving the home cage, being weighed, working on a cognitive task for food reward, and then being returned to the social group.

Training

Positive human-animal interactions can alleviate stress during handling for procedures and help form bonds between animals and care staff [72]. Training macaques to understand and perform certain tasks required of them, using positive reinforcement methods, will provide mental stimulation during the process. New skills and tasks should be integrated into the training programme to keep the animal’s interest. See Habituation and training for more information.

 

A cynomolgus macaque attempts to open a full pumpkin with her infant clinging to her

Food-based

Macaques will voluntarily work for food, even when it is freely available, which indicates that the act of foraging in itself has intrinsic appeal. Feeding macaques should not, therefore, just be seen as satisfying a physical need.

Providing food as a single meal on a predictable schedule, or as easy to process food items in one location (e.g pellets in a hopper), ignores the complex behaviour and cognition associated with feeding, and may have adverse consequences for animal welfare. Appropriate nutrition is clearly essential (see Feeding), but providing variety in the content of the diet and in the method of food presentation will enrich the lives of captive macaques [12].

  • Create foraging opportunities which increase the time and effort required for retrieval of food. This will help stimulate natural foraging behaviour, as well as cognitive processes, and acts as a reward mechanism [72]. 



    Examples include:

    • Scattering food amongst a substrate, such as woodwool or sawdust, either on the floor or inside a paper bag [74].
    • Feeding a very fine forage mix (e.g. a mixture of peanuts in the shell, sunflower seeds, pumpkin seeds, pine kernels, flaked maize, locust beans, banana chips, dried apricots, raisins, rice grains).
    • Putting food items inside containers, either stoppered with something easy-to-remove (e.g. woodwool) or with a lid that must be peeled or screwed off.
    • Utilising puzzle feeders, which require manipulation and the development of fine motor skills for retrieval of food [75,76].
    • Suspending food (e.g. browse) within the enclosure; this will make feeding more challenging and introduce unpredictable movement to the process.
  • Spread out feeding over a number of times during the day.
  • Vary the types of food provided per day and between days.
  • Provide whole food items that require manipulation and processing, as well as food cut into smaller portions.
  • Smear sticky foods (e.g. honey, apple syrup) on surfaces, such as toys or objects which can be hung from the outside of the enclosure or climbing surfaces. Tennis balls can be rolled in syrup or diluted fruit juices, and then into forage mix.
  • Feed live insects. Mealworms are commonly given to captive monkeys, but crickets are also a great source of feeding enrichment as the monkeys must work harder to catch them.
  • Freeze liquids, such as fruit juice or diluted yoghurt, into blocks. Kong toys can also be filled with food (e.g. peanuts, fruit, sunflower seeds) and frozen.

While many novel enrichment items are often presented with food to ease any uncertainty regarding their presentation, it is important to note that not all enrichment items need to be associated with food in order to elicit interest. If an enrichment item is utilised by the macaques without the presence of food, it is recommended that any association with food is avoided if it is suspected that the use of the item would decrease or stop after presentation with food.

In this video, two pair housed rhesus macaques socially forage through sawdust in their pen for a low calorie fine forage mix that their technician has prepared. 

Recipe for a low calorie forage mix

Ingredients

  • Millet seeds – 5 cups
  • Hemp seeds – 3 cups
  • Rice – 3 cups
  • Sesame seeds – 2 cups
  • Split peas or similar – 1/2 cup
  • Sunflower or pumpkin seeds – 1/2 cup

Notes

  • A low calorie, fine forage mix is useful for promoting calm foraging behaviour in groups of macaques housed indoors, even those whose main diet is controlled for a research purpose or health reasons (e.g. obesity)
  • 10g per animal (approx. 35 calories) can be scattered into floor substrate once or twice per day

Recipe for a forage mix

Ingredients

  • Porridge oats
  • Marrowfat peas
  • Red lentils
  • Bran flakes
  • Mustard seeds
  • Safflower seeds
  • Millet
  • Bulgar wheat
  • Barley
  • Desiccated coconut
  • Irradiated wheat
  • Linseed
  • Caraway seeds
  • Coriander
  • Long grain rice
  • Cornflakes
  • Popcorn
  • Sunflower seeds
  • Buckwheat
  • Beans
  • Dun peas
  • Maple peas
  • French maize
  • Yellow peas
  • Dried green peas
  • Cabbage seed
  • Red dari
  • White dari

Notes

  • Some of these ingredients can be found pre-mixed in pigeon seed.
  • Mix up different combinations for variety
  • Forage mix can be used as part of the daily diet (thrown into foraging substrate) or can be used in other types of food enrichment (see below)
Forage mix in a technicians hand. There is a variety of seeds, lentils, wheat, rice etc.
Forage mix(Image: C Kemp/MRC Centre for Macaques)
Adult male foraging for seeds.
Paper bags can be filled with small food items (e.g. seeds, cereals, popcorn) to provide low calorie foraging opportunities.

Recipe for forage bags/packets

Ingredients

  • Paper bags (various sizes)
  • Forage mix
  • Straw/shredded paper

Notes

  • Ensure that the bags have no staples
  • Stuff the bags with straw/shredded paper and place a handful of forage mix inside
  • Roll the bags up tightly so that the monkeys must work a little to open them
  • Ensure that you have enough bags for the number of monkeys to avoid fights
  • Place the bags around the housing and find hiding places
Food items can be mixed with hay or wood shavings and placed inside packaging material to encourage exploration and foraging, after which the macaques may spend time destroying the material.
Whole oranges can be provided to macaques as food enrichment.

 

Macaques can enjoy a variety of fruit as food enrichment. In this video, a macaque eats a melon (Credit: MRC Centre for Macaques).

 

Food based enrichment can be paired with structural enrichment to create a stimulating experience for the macaque. Here, small food items have been placed on a tyre swing allowing the monkey to enhance its skills in balance and manipulation.

 

A macaque forages suspended dates (Credit: BFC).

References

  1. Maple TL and Stine WW (1982). Environmental variables and great ape husbandry. American Journal of Primatology 3(S1): 67-76. https://doi.org/10.1002/ajp.1350030513

  2. Wilson SF (1982). Environmental influences on the activity of captive apes. Zoo Biology 1(3): 201-9. https://doi.org/10.1002/zoo.1430010304

  3. Perkins LA (1992). Variables that influence the activity of captive orangutans. Zoo Biology 11(3): 177-86. https://doi.org/10.1002/zoo.1430110306

  4. Newberry RC (1995). Environmental enrichment: Increasing the biological relevance of captive environments. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 44(2-4): 229-43. https://doi.org/10.1016/0168-1591(95)00616-Z

  5. Shepherdson DJ (1998). Tracing the path of environmental enrichment in zoos`. In: Second nature: Environmental enrichment for captive animals (Eds. Shepherdson DJ, Mellen JD, and Hutchins M) (pp. 1-12). Smithsonian Institution Press.

  6. Kerl J and Roth H (1996). Influence of cage size and cage equipment on physiology and behavior of common marmosets (callithrix jacchus). Laboratory Primate Newsletter 35(3): 10-3.

  7. Kitchen AM and Martin AA (1996). The effects of cage size and complexity on the behaviour of captive common marmosets, callithrix jacchus jacchus. Laboratory Animals 30(4): 317-26. https://doi.org/10.1258/002367796780739853

  8. Jensvold MLA et al. (2001). Effect of enclosure size and complexity on the behaviors of captive chimpanzees (pan troglodytes). Journal of Applied Animal Welfare Science 4(1): 53-69. https://doi.org/10.1207/S15327604JAWS0401_3

  9. Bassett L and Buchanan-Smith HM (2007). Effects of predictability on the welfare of captive animals. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 102(3-4): 223-45. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.applanim.2006.05.029

  10. Buchanan-Smith HM (2010). Environmental enrichment for primates in laboratories. Advances in Science and Research 5(1): 41-56. https://doi.org/10.5194/asr-5-41-2010

  11. Young RJ (2003). Environmental enrichment for captive animals. Blackwell Science.

  12. Jennings M et al. (2009). Refinements in husbandry, care and common procedures for non-human primates. Laboratory Animals 43(1_suppl): 1-47. https://doi.org/10.1258/la.2008.007143

  13. Sambrook TD and Buchanan-Smith HM (1996). What makes novel objects enriching? A comparison of the qualities of control and complexity. Laboratory Primate Newsletter 35(4): 1-4.

  14. Colahan H and Breder C (2003). Primate training at disney's animal kingdom. Journal of Applied Animal Welfare Science 6(3): 235-46. https://doi.org/10.1207/S15327604JAWS0603_08

  15. Lutz CK and Novak MA (2005). Environmental enrichment for nonhuman primates: Theory and application. ILAR Journal 46(2): 178-91. https://doi.org/10.1093/ilar.46.2.178

  16. Buchanan-Smith HM (2010). Environmental enrichment for primates in laboratories. Advances in Science and Research 5(1): 41-56. https://doi.org/10.5194/asr-5-41-2010

  17. Bloomstrand M et al. (1986). Objective evaluation of a behavioral enrichment device for captive chimpanzees (pan troglodytes). Zoo Biology 5(3): 293-300. https://doi.org/10.1002/zoo.1430050307

  18. Schapiro SJ and Bloomsmith MA (1995). Behavioral effects of enrichment on singly-housed, yearling rhesus monkeys: An analysis including three enrichment conditions and a control group. American Journal of Primatology 35(2): 89-101. https://doi.org/10.1002/ajp.1350350202

  19. Baker KC et al. (2014). Comparing options for pair housing rhesus macaques using behavioral welfare measures. American Journal of Primatology 76(1): 30-42. https://doi.org/10.1002/ajp.22190

  20. Crockett C et al. (1989). Kong toys as enrichment devices for singly-caged macaques. Laboratory Primate Newsletter 28: 21-2.

  21. Kessel AL and Brent L (1998). Cage toys reduce abnormal behavior in individually housed pigtail macaques. Journal of Applied Animal Welfare Science 1(3): 227-34. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15327604jaws0103_3

  22. Reinhardt V and Reinhardt A (2008). Environmental enrichment and refinement for nonhuman primates kept in research laboratories: A photographic documentation and literature review. Animal Welfare Institute.

  23. Novak MA et al. (1993). Old, socially housed rhesus monkeys manipulate objects. Zoo Biology 12(3): 285-98. https://doi.org/10.1002/zoo.1430120306

  24. Parks KA and Novak MA (1993). Observations of increased activity and tool use in captive rhesus monkeys exposed to troughs of water. American Journal of Primatology 29(1): 13-25. https://doi.org/10.1002/ajp.1350290103

  25. Hoy JM et al. (2009). Thirty years later: Enrichment practices for captive mammals. Zoo Biology 29(3): 303-16. https://doi.org/10.1002/zoo.20254

  26. Lindburg DG (1971). The rhesus monkey in north india : An ecological and behavioral study. In: Primate behavior: Developments in field and laboratory research (Ed., Rosenblum LA) (Vol. 2, pp. 1-106). Academic Pr.

  27. Hutchins M and Barash DP (1976). Grooming in primates: Implications for its utilitarian function. Primates 17: 145-50.

  28. Reinhardt V (1989). Behavioral responses of unrelated adult male rhesus monkeys familiarized and paired for the purpose of environmental enrichment. American Journal of Primatology 17(3): 243-8. https://doi.org/10.1002/ajp.1350170305

  29. Gust DA et al. (1994). Effect of a preferred companion in modulating stress in adult female rhesus monkeys. Physiology & Behavior 55(4): 681-4. https://doi.org/10.1016/0031-9384(94)90044-2

  30. Sackett GP et al. (1981). Social isolation rearing effects in monkeys vary with genotype. Developmental Psychology 17(3): 313-8. https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.17.3.313

  31. Line SW et al. (1991). Simple toys do not alter the behavior of aged rhesus monkeys. Zoo Biology 10(6): 473-84. https://doi.org/10.1002/zoo.1430100606

  32. Novak MA et al. (1993). Old, socially housed rhesus monkeys manipulate objects. Zoo Biology 12(3): 285-98. https://doi.org/10.1002/zoo.1430120306

  33. Reinhardt V and Reinhardt A (2002). Comfortable quarters for primates in research institutions. Animal Welfare Institute.

  34. Roberts SJ and Platt ML (2005). Effects of isosexual pair-housing on biomedical implants and study participation in male macaques. JAALAS(6): 13-8.

  35. Reinhardt V et al. (1995). Social housing of previously single-caged macaques: What are the options and the risks? Animal Welfare 4(4): 307-28.

  36. Watson LM (2002). A successful program for same- and cross-age pair-housing adult and subadult male macaca fasicularis. Laboratory Primate Newsletter 41: 6-9.

  37. Turner PV et al. (2003). Refinements in the care and use of animals in toxicology studies—regulation, validation, and progress. JAALAS 42(6): 8-15.

  38. DiVincenti Jr L and Wyatt JD (2011). Pair housing of macaques in research facilities: A science-based review of benefits and risks. JAALAS 50(6): 856-63.

  39. Crockett CM et al. (1997). Grooming-contact bars provide social contact for individually caged laboratory macaques. JAALAS 36(6): 53-60.

  40. Carlson J (2008). Safe pair housing of macaques. Animal Welfare Institute.

  41. Rennie AE and Buchanan-Smith HM (2006). Refinement of the use of non-human primates in scientific research. Part ii: Housing, husbandry and acquisition. Animal Welfare. https://www.researchgate.net/publication/228670741

  42. Baker KC et al. (2014). Comparing options for pair housing rhesus macaques using behavioral welfare measures. American Journal of Primatology 76(1): 30-42. https://doi.org/10.1002/ajp.22190

  43. Visalberghil E and Anderson JR (1993). Reasons and risks associated with manipulating captive primates' social environments. Animal Welfare 13: 3-15.

  44. Hartner M et al. (2001). Group-housing subadult male cynomolgus macaques in a pharmaceutical environment. Laboratory Animals 8: 53-7.

  45. Wolfensohn S (2004). Social housing of large primates: Methodology for refinement of husbandry and management. Alternatives to Laboratory Animals 32(1_suppl): 149-51. https://doi.org/10.1177/026119290403201s24

  46. McCowan B et al. (2008). Utility of social network analysis for primate behavioral management and well-being. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 109(2-4): 396-405. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.applanim.2007.02.009

  47. Reinhardt V (1994). Pair-housing rather than single-housing for laboratory rhesus macaques. Journal of Medical Primatology 23(8): 426-31. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1600-0684.1994.tb00131.x

  48. Bryant CE et al. (1988). Effects of different environmental enrichment devices on cage stereotypies and autoaggression in captive cynomolgus monkeys. Journal of Medical Primatology 17(5): 257-69. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1600-0684.1988.tb00388.x

  49. Kopecky J and Reinhardt V (1991). Comparing the effectiveness of pvc swings versus pvc perches as environmental enrichment objects for caged female rhesus macaques (macaca mulatta). Laboratory Primate Newsletter 30(2): 5-6.

  50. Dexter SL and Bayne K (1994). Results of providing swings to individually housed rhesus monkeys (macaca mulatta). Laboratory Primate Newsletter 33(2): 9-12.

  51. Reinhardt V and Reinhardt A (2008). Environmental enrichment and refinement for nonhuman primates kept in research laboratories: A photographic documentation and literature review. Animal Welfare Institute.

  52. Wells DL (2009). Sensory stimulation as environmental enrichment for captive animals: A review. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 118(1-2): 1-11. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.applanim.2009.01.002

  53. Butler RA (1954). Incentive conditions which influence visual exploration. Journal of Experimental Psychology 48(1): 19-23. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0063578

  54. Butler RA (1958). The differential effect of visual and auditory incentives on the performance of monkeys. The American Journal of Psychology 71(3): 591-. https://doi.org/10.2307/1420258

  55. Butler RA (1961). The responsiveness of rhesus monkeys to motion pictures. The Journal of Genetic Psychology 98(2): 239-45. https://doi.org/10.1080/00221325.1961.10534374

  56. Platt DM and Novak MA (1997). Videostimulation as enrichment for captive rhesus monkeys (macaca mulatta). Applied Animal Behaviour Science 52(1-2): 139-55. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0168-1591(96)01093-3

  57. Swartz KB and Rosenblum LA (1980). Operant responding by bonnet macaques for color videotape recordings of social stimuli. Animal Learning & Behavior 8(2): 311-21. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03199611

  58. Haude RH and Detwiler DH (1976). Visual observing by rhesus monkeys: Influence of potentially threatening stimuli. Perceptual and Motor Skills 43(1): 231-7. https://doi.org/10.2466/pms.1976.43.1.231

  59. Pfingst BE et al. (1978). Pure tone thresholds for the rhesus monkey. Hearing Research 1(1): 43-7. https://doi.org/10.1016/0378-5955(78)90008-4

  60. Line SW et al. (1990). Responses of female rhesus macaques to an environmental enrichment apparatus. Laboratory Animals 24(3): 213-20. https://doi.org/10.1258/002367790780866245

  61. Howell S et al. (2003). A stereo music system as environmental enrichment for captive chimpanzees. Lab Animal 32(10): 31-6. https://doi.org/10.1038/laban1103-31

  62. Ogden JJ et al. (1994). A preliminary study of the effects of ecologically relevant sounds on the behaviour of captive lowland gorillas. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 39(2): 163-76. https://doi.org/10.1016/0168-1591(94)90136-8

  63. Patterson-Kane EG and Farnworth MJ (2006). Noise exposure, music, and animals in the laboratory: A commentary based on laboratory animal refinement and enrichment forum (laref) discussions. Journal of Applied Animal Welfare Science 9(4): 327-32. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15327604jaws0904_7

  64. Laska M (2000). 'Microsmatic' primates revisited: Olfactory sensitivity in the squirrel monkey. Chemical Senses 25(1): 47-53. https://doi.org/10.1093/chemse/25.1.47

  65. Anderson JR et al. (1992). Task-directed and recreational underwater swimming in captive rhesus monkeys (macaca mulatta). Laboratory Primate Newsletter 31(4): 1-4.

  66. Anderson JR et al. (1994). Diving and underwater swimming as enrichment activities for captive rhesus macaques (macaca mulatta). Animal Welfare 3(4): 275-83.

  67. Parks KA and Novak MA (1993). Observations of increased activity and tool use in captive rhesus monkeys exposed to troughs of water. American Journal of Primatology 29(1): 13-25. https://doi.org/10.1002/ajp.1350290103

  68. Robins JG and Waitt CD (2011). Improving the welfare of captive macaques (macaca sp) through the use of water as enrichment. Journal of Applied Animal Welfare Science 14(1): 75-84. https://doi.org/10.1080/10888705.2011.527605

  69. Whitehouse J et al. (2013). The impact of cognitive testing on the welfare of group housed primates. PLoS ONE 8(11): e78308-e. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0078308

  70. Fagot J and Paleressompoulle D (2009). Automatic testing of cognitive performance in baboons maintained in social groups. Behavior Research Methods 41(2): 396-404. https://doi.org/10.3758/BRM.41.2.396

  71. Clark FE (2011). Great ape cognition and captive care: Can cognitive challenges enhance well-being? Applied Animal Behaviour Science 135(1-2): 1-12. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.applanim.2011.10.010

  72. Buchanan-Smith HM (2010). Environmental enrichment for primates in laboratories. Advances in Science and Research 5(1): 41-56. https://doi.org/10.5194/asr-5-41-2010

  73. Reinhardt V (2000). Wooden objects for enrichment: A discussion. Laboratory Primate Newsletter 39(3): 1-4.

  74. Byrne GD and Suomi SJ (1991). Effects of woodchips and buried food on behavior patterns and psychological well-being of captive rhesus monkeys. American Journal of Primatology 23(3): 141-51. https://doi.org/10.1002/ajp.1350230302

  75. Bloom KR and Cook M (1989). Environment enrichment: Behavioral response of rhesus to puzzle feeders.

  76. Heath S et al. (1992). Peanut puzzle solvers quickly demonstrate aptitude. Laboratory Primate Newsletter 31(1): 12-3.